anti-constructivist article

I wrote about the article...
The rough idea I get is that is that "project based learning" should not be a "Here, kids, try these project" and the teacher hopes the kid will "discover" the new information.

I like the definition of learning:     Learning, in turn, is defined as a change in long-term memory.

-- Steve


REPLY FROM Dennis Yuzenas, a project-based learning teacher and webmaster of WhatDoyaKnow.com
Steve,
Great article. I've run into the problem of how to get stuff into kids' heads that will enable me to get a product at the end of the learning process that is authentic and can legitimately be called "education." My class is a hybrid.
 
I think, if we're serious about approaching every learner as a unique person with different learning styles and various emotional components that must be addressed in the process, that many assertions made in the article must be addressed in the lesson design phase.
 
The following is unsupportable (FROM THE ARTICLE): Minimally guided instruction appears to proceed with no reference to the characteristics of working memory, long-term memory, or the intricate relations between them. The result is a series of recommendations that most educators find almost impossible to implement—and many experienced educators are reluctant to implement—because they require learners to engage in cognitive activities that are highly unlikely to result in effective learning.  Then the next paragraph further asserts: Sensory memory is not relevant to the discussion here so it is not considered further.
 
It appears to me that someone has set out to rip problem-based instruction and propel the notion of the superiority of the drill and kill method of instruction favored by politicians and educational hacks.
 
Both statements above are totally false. For project-based learning to work there must be an amazing amount of preplanning done on the part of the teacher and the learner. There is a critical mass of information students must posess before they are launched into their project. This information is imparted using sensory memory as much as possible. This is the information that gets "seared" into the brain!
 
If all we want is the memorization of random facts then this article is right on. I think Daniel Pink would have something to say about that...
 
We are skillful in an area because our long-term memory contains huge amounts of information concerning the area. Why does the author of this article think project-based learning doesn't require information stored in our long-term memory? Isn't the whole purpose behind employing pbl making things that are to be learned a part of long term memory? The author states: Any instructional recommendation that does not or cannot specify what has been changed in long-term memory, or that does not increase the efficiency with which relevant information is stored in or retrieved from long-term memory, is likely to be ineffective. 
 
Another invalid, totally bogus assertion: Recommendations advocating minimal guidance during instruction proceed as though working memory does not exist or, if it does exist, that it has no relevant limitations when dealing with novel information, the very information of interest to constructivist teaching procedures.
 
All problem-based searching makes heavy demands on working memory. Furthermore, that working memory load does not contribute to the accumulation of knowledge in long-term memory because while working memory is being used to search for problem solutions, it is not available and cannot be used to learn. What? This is bullshit, pure and simple.
 
The consequences of requiring novice learners to search for problem solutions using a limited working memory or the mechanisms by which unguided or minimally guided instruction might facilitate change in long-term memory appear to be routinely ignored. The result is a set of differently named but similar instructional approaches requiring minimal guidance that are disconnected from much that we know of human cognition. Unsupported assertion after unsupported assertion.
 
Then the author goes on to slam Bruner.
 
Bruner says we should build on prior knowledge. If you're really into pbl then you're going to bring Vygotsky in about now. He says we should push our students to the edge of frustration in presenting information--knowledge--to students and make them work with it. Hence--a novel product! Throw Glaser into the mix--people want to control their lives. Add some Gardner and Pink--and you have the pbl classroom.
 
D. Yuzenas
 
 

 


From: Steve McCrea <happymathteacher@gmail.com>
To: yuzenasdennis@bellsouth.net
Cc: jwhutt@yahoo.comfsavain@cofrafinancial.net; Matthew Blazek <mblazek@mavericksineducation.com>
Sent: Fri, June 17, 2011 10:09:47 AM
Subject: Anti-constructivism dfhttp://pdfserve.informaworld.com/101737__784754045.pdf


http://pdfserve.informaworld.com/101737__784754045.pdf

the full article

Downloaded At: 14:04 17 June 201176 KIRSCHNER, SWELLER, CLARK

Correspondence should be addressed to Paul A. Kirschner, Research Centre Learning in Interaction, Utrecht University, The Netherlands, P.O. Box 80140, 3508 TC, Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail:p.a.kirschner@fss.uu.nl


Evidence for the superiority of guided instruction is explained in the context of our knowledge of human cognitive architecture, expert–novice differences, and cognitive load. Although un- guided or minimally guided instructional approaches are very popular and intuitively appeal- ing, the point is made that these approaches ignore both the structures that constitute human cognitive architecture and evidence from empirical studies over the past half-century that con- sistently indicate that minimally guided instruction is less effective and less efficient than in- structional approaches that place a strong emphasis on guidance of the student learning pro- cess. The advantage of guidance begins to recede only when learners have sufficiently high prior knowledge to provide “internal” guidance. Recent developments in instructional research and instructional design models that support guidance during instruction are briefly described.
Disputes about the impact of instructional guidance during teaching have been ongoing for at least the past half-century (Ausubel, 1964; Craig, 1956; Mayer, 2004; Shulman & Keisler, 1966). On one side of this argument are those advocating the hypothesis that people learn best in an unguided or minimally guided environment, generally defined as one in which learners, rather than being presented with essential in- formation, must discover or construct essential information for themselves (e.g., Bruner, 1961; Papert, 1980; Steffe & Gale, 1995). On the other side are those suggesting that nov- ice learners should be provided with direct instructional guidance on the concepts and procedures required by a par- ticular discipline and should not be left to discover those procedures by themselves (e.g., Cronbach & Snow, 1977; Klahr & Nigam, 2004; Mayer, 2004; Shulman & Keisler, 1966; Sweller, 2003). Direct instructional guidance is defined as providing information that fully explains the concepts and procedures that students are required to learn as well as learn- ing strategy support that is compatible with human cognitive architecture. Learning, in turn, is defined as a change in long-term memory.
The minimally guided approach has been called by vari- ous names including discovery learning (Anthony, 1973; Bruner, 1961); problem-based learning (PBL; Barrows & Tamblyn, 1980; Schmidt, 1983), inquiry learning (Papert, 1980; Rutherford, 1964), experiential learning (Boud, Keogh, & Walker, 1985; Kolb & Fry, 1975), and constructivist learning (Jonassen, 1991; Steffe & Gale, 1995). Examples of applications of these differently named but essentially pedagogically equivalent approaches include
science instruction in which students are placed in inquiry learning contexts and asked to discover the fundamental and well-known principles of science by modeling the investiga- tory activities of professional researchers (Van Joolingen, de Jong, Lazonder, Savelsbergh, & Manlove, 2005). Similarly, medical students in problem-based teaching courses are re- quired to discover medical solutions for common patient problems using problem-solving techniques (Schmidt, 1998, 2000).
There seem to be two main assumptions underlying in- structional programs using minimal guidance. First they chal- lenge students to solve “authentic” problems or acquire com- plex knowledge in information-rich settings based on the assumption that having learners construct their own solutions leads to the most effective learning experience. Second, they appear to assume that knowledge can best be acquired through experience based on the procedures of the discipline (i.e., see- ing the pedagogic content of the learning experience as identi- cal to the methods and processes or epistemology of the disci- pline being studied; Kirschner, 1992). Minimal guidance is offered in the form of process- or task-relevant information that is available if learners choose to use it. Advocates of this approach imply that instructional guidance that provides or embeds learning strategies in instruction interferes with the natural processes by which learners draw on their unique prior experience and learning styles to construct new situated knowledge that will achieve their goals. According to Wickens (1992, cited in Bernstein, Penner, Clarke-Stewart, Roy, & Wickens, 2003), for example,
large amounts of guidance may produce very good perfor- mance during practice, but too much guidance may impair later performance. Coaching students about correct responses in math, for example, may impair their ability later to retrieve correct responses from memory on their own. (p. 221)
This constructivist argument has attracted a significant fol- lowing.
The goal of this article is to suggest that based on our current knowledge of human cognitive architecture, mini- mally guided instruction is likely to be ineffective. The past half-century of empirical research on this issue has pro- vided overwhelming and unambiguous evidence that mini- mal guidance during instruction is significantly less effec- tive and efficient than guidance specifically designed to support the cognitive processing necessary for learning.

THE CONSEQUENCES OF HUMAN COGNITIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR MINIMAL GUIDANCE DURING INSTRUCTION
Any instructional procedure that ignores the structures that constitute human cognitive architecture is not likely to be ef- fective. Minimally guided instruction appears to proceed
with no reference to the characteristics of working memory, long-term memory, or the intricate relations between them. The result is a series of recommendations that most educators find almost impossible to implement—and many experi- enced educators are reluctant to implement—because they require learners to engage in cognitive activities that are highly unlikely to result in effective learning. As a conse- quence, the most effective teachers may either ignore the rec- ommendations or, at best, pay lip service to them (e.g., Aulls, 2002). In this section we discuss some of the characteristics of human cognitive architecture and the consequent instructional implications.

Human Cognitive Architecture
Human cognitive architecture is concerned with the manner in which our cognitive structures are organized. Most modern treatments of human cognitive architecture use the Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) sensory memory–working mem- ory–long-term memory model as their base. Sensory memory is not relevant to the discussion here so it is not considered fur- ther. The relations between working and long-term memory, in conjunction with the cognitive processes that support learn- ing, are of critical importance to the argument.
Our understanding of the role of long-term memory in hu- man cognition has altered dramatically over the last few de- cades. It is no longer seen as a passive repository of discrete, isolated fragments of information that permit us to repeat what we have learned. Nor is it seen only as a component of human cognitive architecture that has merely peripheral in- fluence on complex cognitive processes such as thinking and problem solving. Rather, long-term memory is now viewed as the central, dominant structure of human cognition. Every- thing we see, hear, and think about is critically dependent on and influenced by our long-term memory.
De Groot’s (1945/1965) work on chess expertise, followed by Chase and Simon (1973), has served as a major influence on the field’s reconceptualization of the role of long-term mem- ory. The finding that expert chess players are far better able than novices to reproduce briefly seen board configurations taken from real games, but do not differ in reproducing random board configurations, has been replicated in a variety of other areas (e.g., Egan & Schwartz, 1979; Jeffries, Turner, Polson, & Atwood, 1981; Sweller & Cooper, 1985). These results sug- gest that expert problem solvers derive their skill by drawing on the extensive experience stored in their long-term memory and then quickly select and apply the best procedures for solv- ing problems. The fact that these differences can be used to fully explain problem-solving skill emphasizes the impor- tance of long-term memory to cognition. We are skillful in an area because our long-term memory contains huge amounts of information concerning the area. That information permits us to quickly recognize the characteristics of a situation and indi- cates to us, often unconsciously, what to do and when to do it. Without our huge store of information in long-term memory,
we would be largely incapable of everything from simple acts such as crossing a street (information in long-term memory informs us how to avoid speeding traffic, a skill many other an- imals are unable to store in their long-term memories) to com- plex activities such as playing chess or solving mathematical problems. Thus, our long-term memory incorporates a mas- sive knowledge base that is central to all of our cognitively based activities.
What are the instructional consequences of long-term memory? In the first instance and at its most basic, the archi- tecture of long-term memory provides us with the ultimate justification for instruction. The aim of all instruction is to al- ter long-term memory. If nothing has changed in long-term memory, nothing has been learned. Any instructional recom- mendation that does not or cannot specify what has been changed in long-term memory, or that does not increase the efficiency with which relevant information is stored in or re- trieved from long-term memory, is likely to be ineffective.

Working Memory Characteristics and Functions
Working memory is the cognitive structure in which con- scious processing occurs. We are only conscious of the infor- mation currently being processed in working memory and are more or less oblivious to the far larger amount of informa- tion stored in long-term memory.
Working memory has two well-known characteristics: When processing novel information, it is very limited in du- ration and in capacity. We have known at least since Peterson and Peterson (1959) that almost all information stored in working memory and not rehearsed is lost within 30 sec and have known at least since Miller (1956) that the capacity of working memory is limited to only a very small number of el- ements. That number is about seven according to Miller, but may be as low as four, plus or minus one (see, e.g., Cowan, 2001). Furthermore, when processing rather than merely storing information, it may be reasonable to conjecture that the number of items that can be processed may only be two or three, depending on the nature of the processing required.
The interactions between working memory and long-term memory may be even more important than the processing limitations (Sweller, 2003, 2004). The limitations of working memory only apply to new, yet to be learned information that has not been stored in long-term memory. New information such as new combinations of numbers or letters can only be stored for brief periods with severe limitations on the amount of such information that can be dealt with. In contrast, when dealing with previously learned information stored in long-term memory, these limitations disappear. In the sense that information can be brought back from long-term mem- ory to working memory over indefinite periods of time, the temporal limits of working memory become irrelevant. Simi- larly, there are no known limits to the amount of such infor- mation that can be brought into working memory from long-term memory. Indeed, the altered characteristics of
working memory when processing familiar as opposed to un- familiar material induced Ericsson and Kintsch (1995) to propose a separate structure, long-term working memory, to deal with well-learned and automated information.
Any instructional theory that ignores the limits of working memory when dealing with novel information or ignores the disappearance of those limits when dealing with familiar in- formation is unlikely to be effective. Recommendations advo- cating minimal guidance during instruction proceed as though working memory does not exist or, if it does exist, that it has no relevant limitations when dealing with novel information, the very information of interest to constructivist teaching proce- dures. We know that problem solving, which is central to one instructional procedure advocating minimal guidance, called inquiry-based instruction, places a huge burden on working memory (Sweller, 1988). The onus should surely be on those who support inquiry-based instruction to explain how such a procedure circumvents the well-known limits of working memory when dealing with novel information.

Implications of Human Cognitive Architecture for Constructivist Instruction
These memory structures and their relations have direct im- plications for instructional design (e.g., Sweller, 1999; Sweller, van Merriënboer & Paas, 1998). Inquiry-based in- struction requires the learner to search a problem space for problem-relevant information. All problem-based searching makes heavy demands on working memory. Furthermore, that working memory load does not contribute to the accu- mulation of knowledge in long-term memory because while working memory is being used to search for problem solu- tions, it is not available and cannot be used to learn. Indeed, it is possible to search for extended periods of time with quite minimal alterations to long-term memory (e.g., see Sweller, Mawer, & Howe, 1982). The goal of instruction is rarely sim- ply to search for or discover information. The goal is to give learners specific guidance about how to cognitively manipu- late information in ways that are consistent with a learning goal, and store the result in long-term memory.
The consequences of requiring novice learners to search for problem solutions using a limited working memory or the mechanisms by which unguided or minimally guided in- struction might facilitate change in long-term memory ap- pear to be routinely ignored. The result is a set of differently named but similar instructional approaches requiring mini- mal guidance that are disconnected from much that we know of human cognition. Recommending minimal guidance was understandable when Bruner (1961) proposed discovery learning as an instructional tool because the structures and relations that constitute human cognitive architecture had not yet been mapped. We now are in a quite different environ- ment because we know much more about the structures, functions, and characteristics of working and long-term memory; the relations between them; and their consequences
for learning and problem solving. This new understanding has been the basis for systematic research and development of instructional theories that reflect our current understand- ing of cognitive architecture (e.g., Anderson, 1996; Glaser, 1987). This work should be central to the design of effective, guided instruction.
Of course, suggestions based on theory that minimally guided instruction should have minimal effectiveness are worth little without empirical evidence. Empirical work comparing guided and unguided instruction is discussed af- ter a review of the current arguments for minimal guidance.

THE ORIGINS OF CONSTRUCTIVISM AND THE CURRENT VIEW OF MINIMALLY GUIDED INSTRUCTION
Given the incompatibility of minimally guided instruction with our knowledge of human cognitive architecture, what has been the justification for these approaches? The most re- cent version of instruction with minimal guidance comes from constructivism (e.g., Steffe & Gale, 1995), which ap- pears to have been derived from observations that knowledge is constructed by learners and so (a) they need to have the op- portunity to construct by being presented with goals and min- imal information, and (b) learning is idiosyncratic and so a common instructional format or strategies are ineffective. The constructivist description of learning is accurate, but the instructional consequences suggested by constructivists do not necessarily follow.
Most learners of all ages know how to construct knowl- edge when given adequate information and there is no evi- dence that presenting them with partial information enhances their ability to construct a representation more than giving them full information. Actually, quite the reverse seems most often to be true. Learners must construct a mental representa- tion or schema irrespective of whether they are given com- plete or partial information. Complete information will result in a more accurate representation that is also more easily ac- quired. Constructivism is based therefore, on an observation that, although descriptively accurate, does not lead to a pre- scriptive instructional design theory or to effective pedagogi- cal techniques (Clark & Estes, 1998, 1999; Estes & Clark, 1999; Kirschner, Martens, & Strijbos, 2004). Yet many edu- cators, educational researchers, instructional designers, and learning materials developers appear to have embraced mini- mally guided instruction and tried to implement it.
Another consequence of attempts to implement constructivist theory is a shift of emphasis away from teaching a discipline as a body of knowledge toward an exclusive em- phasis on learning a discipline by experiencing the processes and procedures of the discipline (Handelsman et. al., 2004; Hodson, 1988). This change in focus was accompanied by an assumption shared by many leading educators and discipline specialists that knowledge can best be learned or only learned through experience that is based primarily on the procedures of the discipline. This point of view led to a commitment by ed- ucators to extensive practical or project work, and the rejection of instruction based on the facts, laws, principles and theories that make up a discipline’s content accompanied by the use of discovery and inquiry methods of instruction. The addition of a more vigorous emphasis on the practical application of in- quiry and problem-solving skills seems very positive. Yet it may be a fundamental error to assume that the pedagogic con- tent of the learning experience is identical to the methods and processes (i.e., the epistemology) of the discipline being stud- ied and a mistake to assume that instruction should exclusively focus on methods and processes.
Shulman (1986; Shulman & Hutchings, 1999) contributed to our understanding of the reason why less guided ap- proaches fail in his discussion of the integration of content expertise and pedagogical skill. He defined content knowl- edge as “the amount and organization of the knowledge per se in the mind of the teacher” (Shulman, 1986, p. 9), and ped- agogical content knowledge as knowledge “which goes be- yond knowledge of subject matter per se to the dimension of subject knowledge for teaching” (p. 9). He further defined curricular knowledge as “the pharmacopoeia from which the teacher draws those tools of teaching that present or exem- plify particular content” (p. 10). Kirschner (1991, 1992) also argued that the way an expert works in his or her domain (epistemology) is not equivalent to the way one learns in that area (pedagogy). A similar line of reasoning was followed by Dehoney (1995), who posited that the mental models and strategies of experts have been developed through the slow process of accumulating experience in their domain areas.
Despite this clear distinction between learning a discipline and practicing a discipline, many curriculum developers, edu- cational technologists, and educators seem to confuse the teaching of a discipline as inquiry (i.e., a curricular emphasis on the research processes within a science) with the teaching of the discipline by inquiry (i.e., using the research process of the discipline as a pedagogy or for learning). The basis of this confusion may lie in what Hurd (1969) called the rationale of the scientist, which holds that a course of instruction in science
should be a mirror image of a science discipline, with regard to both its conceptual structure and its patterns of inquiry. The theories and methods of modern science should be re- flected in the classroom. In teaching a science, classroom op- erations should be in harmony with its investigatory pro- cesses and supportive of the conceptual, the intuitive, and the theoretical structure of its knowledge. (p. 16)
This rationale assumes
that the attainment of certain attitudes, the fostering of inter- est in science, the acquisition of laboratory skills, the learn- ing of scientific knowledge, and the understanding of the na- ture of science were all to be approached through the methodology of science, which was, in general, seen in in- ductive terms. (Hodson, 1988, p. 22)
The major fallacy of this rationale is that it makes no distinc- tion between the behaviors and methods of a researcher who is an expert practicing a profession and those students who are new to the discipline and who are, thus, essentially novices.
According to Kyle (1980), scientific inquiry is a system- atic and investigative performance ability incorporating un- restrained thinking capabilities after a person has acquired a broad, critical knowledge of the particular subject matter through formal teaching processes. It may not be equated with investigative methods of science teaching, self-instruc- tional teaching techniques, or open-ended teaching tech- niques. Educators who confuse the two are guilty of the im- proper use of inquiry as a paradigm on which to base an instructional strategy.
Finally, Novak (1988), in noting that the major effort to improve secondary school science education in the 1950s and 1960s fell short of expectations, went so far as to say that the major obstacle that stood in the way of “revolutionary im- provement of science education ... was the obsolete episte- mology that was behind the emphasis on ‘inquiry’ oriented science” (pp. 79–80).

RESEARCH COMPARING GUIDED AND UNGUIDED INSTRUCTION
None of the preceding arguments and theorizing would be important if there was a clear body of research using con- trolled experiments indicating that unguided or minimally guided instruction was more effective than guided instruc- tion. In fact, precisely as one might expect from our knowl- edge of human cognition and the distinctions between learn- ing and practicing a discipline, the reverse is true. Controlled experiments almost uniformly indicate that when dealing with novel information, learners should be explicitly shown what to do and how to do it.
A number of reviews of empirical studies have established a solid research-based case against the use of instruction with minimal guidance. Although an extensive review of those studies is outside the scope of this article, Mayer (2004) re- cently reviewed evidence from studies conducted from 1950 to the late 1980s comparing pure discovery learning, defined as unguided, problem-based instruction, with guided forms of instruction. He suggested that in each decade since the mid-1950s, when empirical studies provided solid evidence that the then popular unguided approach did not work, a simi- lar approach popped up under a different name with the cycle then repeating itself. Each new set of advocates for unguided approaches seemed either unaware of or uninterested in pre- vious evidence that unguided approaches had not been vali- dated. This pattern produced discovery learning, which gave way to experiential learning, which gave way to prob-
lem-based and inquiry learning, which now gives way to constructivist instructional techniques. Mayer (2004) con- cluded that the “debate about discovery has been replayed many times in education but each time, the evidence has fa- vored a guided approach to learning” (p. 18).

Current Research Supporting Direct Guidance
Because students learn so little from a constructivist approach, most teachers who attempt to implement classroom-based constructivist instruction end up providing students with con- siderable guidance. This is a reasonable interpretation, for ex- ample, of qualitative case studies conducted by Aulls (2002), who observed a number of teachers as they implemented constructivist activities in their classrooms. He described the “scaffolding” that the most effective teachers introduced when students failed to make learning progress in a discovery set- ting. He reported that the teacher whose students achieved all of their learning goals spent a great deal of time in instructional interactions with students by
simultaneously teaching content and scaffolding-relevant procedures ... by (a) modeling procedures for identifying and self-checking important information ... (b) showing stu- dents how to reduce that information to paraphrases ... (c) having students use notes to construct collaborations and routines, and (d) promoting collaborative dialogue within problems. (p. 533)
Stronger evidence from well-designed, controlled experi- mental studies also supports direct instructional guidance (e.g., see Moreno, 2004; Tuovinen & Sweller, 1999). Hardiman, Pollatsek, and Weil (1986) and Brown and Campione (1994) noted that when students learn science in classrooms with pure-discovery methods and minimal feed- back, they often become lost and frustrated, and their confu- sion can lead to misconceptions. Others (e.g., Carlson, Lundy, & Schneider, 1992; Schauble, 1990) found that be- cause false starts are common in such learning situations, un- guided discovery is most often inefficient. Moreno (2004) concluded that there is a growing body of research showing that students learn more deeply from strongly guided learning than from discovery. Similar conclusions were reported by Chall (2000), McKeough, Lupart, and Marini (1995), Schauble (1990), and Singley and Anderson (1989). Klahr and Nigam (2004), in a very important study, not only tested whether science learners learned more via a discovery versus direct instruction route but also, once learning had occurred, whether the quality of learning differed. Specifically, they tested whether those who had learned through discovery were better able to transfer their learning to new contexts. The findings were unambiguous. Direct instruction involving considerable guidance, including examples, resulted in vastly more learning than discovery. Those relatively few
students who learned via discovery showed no signs of superior quality of learning.

Cognitive load. Sweller and others (Mayer, 2001; Paas, Renkl, & Sweller, 2003, 2004; Sweller, 1999, 2004; Winn, 2003) noted that despite the alleged advantages of un- guided environments to help students to derive meaning from learning materials, cognitive load theory suggests that the free exploration of a highly complex environment may gen- erate a heavy working memory load that is detrimental to learning. This suggestion is particularly important in the case of novice learners, who lack proper schemas to integrate the new information with their prior knowledge. Tuovinen and Sweller (1999) showed that exploration practice (a discovery technique) caused a much larger cognitive load and led to poorer learning than worked-examples practice. The more knowledgeable learners did not experience a negative effect and benefited equally from both types of treatments. Mayer (2001) described an extended series of experiments in multi- media instruction that he and his colleagues have designed drawing on Sweller’s (1988, 1999) cognitive load theory and other cognitively based theoretical sources. In all of the many studies he reported, guided instruction not only produced more immediate recall of facts than unguided approaches, but also longer term transfer and problem-solving skills.
Worked examples. A worked example constitutes the epitome of strongly guided instruction, whereas discovering the solution to a problem in an information-rich environment similarly constitutes the epitome of minimally guided dis- covery learning. The worked-example effect, which is based on cognitive load theory, occurs when learners required to solve problems perform worse on subsequent test problems than learners who study the equivalent worked examples. Ac- cordingly, the worked-example effect, which has been repli- cated a number of times, provides some of the strongest evi- dence for the superiority of directly guided instruction over minimal guidance. The fact that the effect relies on con- trolled experiments adds to its importance.
The worked-example effect was first demonstrated by Sweller and Cooper (1985) and Cooper and Sweller (1987), who found that algebra students learned more studying alge- bra worked examples than solving the equivalent problems. Since those early demonstrations of the effect, it has been replicated on numerous occasions using a large variety of learners studying an equally large variety of materials (Carroll, 1994; Miller, Lehman, & Koedinger, 1999; Paas, 1992; Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994; Pillay, 1994; Quilici & Mayer, 1996; Trafton & Reiser, 1993). For novices, studying worked examples seems invariably superior to discovering or constructing a solution to a problem.
Why does the worked-example effect occur? It can be ex- plained by cognitive load theory, which is grounded in the hu- man cognitive architecture discussed earlier. Solving a prob- lem requires problem-solving search and search must occur
using our limited working memory. Problem-solving search is an inefficient way of altering long-term memory because its function is to find a problem solution, not alter long-term memory. Indeed, problem-solving search can function per- fectly with no learning whatsoever (Sweller, 1988). Thus, problem-solving search overburdens limited working mem- ory and requires working memory resources to be used for ac- tivities that are unrelated to learning. As a consequence, learn- ers can engage in problem-solving activities for extended periods and learn almost nothing (Sweller et al., 1982).
In contrast, studying a worked example both reduces working memory load because search is reduced or elimi- nated and directs attention (i.e., directs working memory re- sources) to learning the essential relations between prob- lem-solving moves. Students learn to recognize which moves are required for particular problems, the basis for the acquisi- tion of problem-solving schemas (Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982). When compared to students who have solved prob- lems rather than studied worked examples, the consequence is the worked-example effect.
There are conditions under which the worked-example ef- fect is not obtainable. First, it is not obtainable when the worked examples are themselves structured in a manner that imposes a heavy cognitive load. In other words, it is quite possible to structure worked examples in a manner that im- poses as heavy a cognitive load as attempting to learn by dis- covering a problem solution (Tarmizi & Sweller, 1988; Ward & Sweller, 1990). Second, the worked-example effect first disappears and then reverses as the learners’ expertise in- creases. Problem solving only becomes relatively effective when learners are sufficiently experienced so that studying a worked example is, for them, a redundant activity that in- creases working memory load compared to generating a known solution (Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001). This phenomenon is an example of the expertise re- versal effect (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003). It emphasizes the importance of providing novices in an area with extensive guidance because they do not have sufficient knowledge in long-term memory to prevent unproductive problem-solving search. That guidance can be relaxed only with increased expertise as knowledge in long-term memory can take over from external guidance.

Process worksheets. Another way of guiding instruc- tion is the use of process worksheets (Van Merriënboer, 1997). Such worksheets provide a description of the phases one should go through when solving the problem as well as hints or rules of thumb that may help to successfully complete each phase. Students can consult the process worksheet while they are working on the learning tasks and they may use it to note in- termediate results of the problem-solving process.
Nadolski, Kirschner, and van Merriënboer (2005), for ex- ample, studied the effects of process worksheets with law students and found that the availability of a process worksheet had positive effects on learning task performance,
Downloaded At: 14:04 17 June 2011
indicated by a higher coherence and more accurate content of the legal case being developed. Learners receiving guidance through process worksheets outperformed learners left to discover the appropriate procedures themselves.

RESEARCH ON EDUCATIONAL MODELS FAVORING MINIMAL GUIDANCE DURING INSTRUCTION IN VARIOUS SETTINGS
Having discussed both the human cognitive architecture re- sponsible for learning and current research supporting direct instruction through guidance, this section discusses a number of the alternative educational models that see and use mini- mal guidance as an approach to learning and instruction.

Experiential Learning at Work
Kolb (1971) and Kolb and Fry (1975) argued that the learning process often begins with a person carrying out a particular action and then seeing or discovering the effect of the action in this situation. The second step is to understand these ef- fects in the particular instance so that if the same action was taken in the same circumstances it would be possible to antic- ipate what would follow from the action. Using this pattern, the third step would be to understand the general principle under which the particular instance falls. They also sug- gested a number of learning styles that they hypothesized would influence the way that students took advantage of ex- periential situations.
Attempts to validate experiential learning and learning styles (Kolb, 1971, 1984, 1999) appear not to have been com- pletely successful. Iliff (1994), for example, reported in “a meta-analysis of 101 quantitative LSI studies culled from 275 dissertations and 624 articles that were qualitative, theo- retical, and quantitative studies of ELT and the Kolb Learning Style Inventory” (Kolb, Boyatzis, & Mainemelis, 2001, p. 20) correlations classified as low (< .5) and effect sizes that were weak (.2) to medium (.5). He concluded that the magnitude of these statistics is not sufficient to meet stan- dards of predictive validity to support the use of the measures or the experiential methods for training at work. Similarly, Ruble and Stout (1993), citing a number of studies from 1980 through 1991, concluded that the Kolb Learning Style Inven- tory (KLSI-1976; Kolb, 1976) has low test–retest reliability, that there is little or no correlation between factors that should correlate with the classification of learning styles, and that it does not enjoy a general acceptance of its usefulness, particularly for research purposes.
Roblyer (1996) and Perkins (1991) examined evidence for minimally guided pedagogy in instructional design and in- structional technology studies. Both researchers concluded that the available evidence does not support the use of mini- mal guidance and both suggested that some form of stronger guidance is necessary for both effective learning and transfer.

Individual Differences in Learning From Instruction
Constructivist approaches to instruction are based, in part, on a concern that individual differences moderate the impact of instruction. This concern has been shared by a large body of aptitude–treatment interaction (ATI) studies that examine whether the effects of different instructional methods are influenced by student aptitudes and traits (e.g., Cronbach & Snow, 1977; Kyllonen & Lajoie, 2003; Snow, Corno, & Jack- son, 1996). Much of this work provides a clear antecedent to the expertise reversal effect, discussed earlier, according to which instructional methods that are effective for novices be- come less effective as expertise increases.
Cronbach and Snow’s (1977) review of ATI research de- scribed a number of replicated ordinal and disordinal interac- tions between various instructional methods and aptitudes. One of the most common ATI findings according to Kyllonen and Lajoie (2003) was “that strong treatments benefited less able learners and weaker treatments benefited more able learners” (p. 82). This conclusion anticipated the now recog- nized scaffolding effect.
In the instructional methods described by Cronbach and Snow (1977) strong treatments implied highly structured in- structional presentations where explicit organization of in- formation and learning support were provided. The weaker treatments were relatively unstructured and so provided much less learning support. The aptitude measures used in the research reviewed by Cronbach and Snow were varied but usually involved some measure of specific subject matter knowledge and measures of crystallized and fluid ability. Snow and Lohman (1984) encouraged research that attempts to understand the cognitive processes demanded by specific learning goals. They argued for a concern with describing the cognitive processes required to learn specific classes of tasks, how those processes are reflected in learner aptitudes, and how characteristics of instructional treatments might compensate for students with lower relevant aptitude by provid- ing needed cognitive processes to help them achieve learning and transfer goals.

Knowing Less After Instruction
A related set of findings in the ATI research paradigm was described by Clark (1989). He reviewed approximately 70 ATI studies and described a number of experiments in which lower aptitude students who choose or were assigned to un- guided, weaker instructional treatments receive significantly lower scores on posttests than on pretest measures. He ar- gued that the failure to provide strong learning support for less experienced or less able students could actually produce a measurable loss of learning. The educational levels repre- sented in the studies reviewed ranged from elementary class- rooms to university and work settings and included a variety of types of problems and tasks. Even more distressing is the
evidence Clark (1982) presented that when learners are asked to select between a more or a less unguided version of the same course, less able learners who choose less guided ap- proaches tend to like the experience even though they learn less from it. Higher aptitude students who chose highly struc- tured approaches tended to like them but achieve at a lower level than with less structured versions but did not suffer by knowing less after than before instruction. Clark hypothe- sized that the most effective components of treatments help less experienced learners by providing task-specific learning strategies embedded in instructional presentations. These strategies require explicit, attention-driven effort on the part of learners and so tend not to be liked, even though they are helpful to learning. More able learners, he suggested, have acquired implicit, task-specific learning strategies that are more effective for them than those embedded in the struc- tured versions of the course. Clark pointed to suggestive evi- dence that more able students who select the more guided versions of courses do so because they believe that they will achieve the required learning with a minimum of effort. Studies described by Woltz (2003) are a recent and positive example of ATI research that examines the cognitive process- ing required for learning tasks. He provided evidence that the same learner might benefit from stronger and weaker treat- ments depending on the type of learning and transfer out- come desired.

Empirical Evidence About Science Learning From Unguided Instruction
The work of Klahr and Nigam (2004), discussed earlier, un- ambiguously demonstrated the advantages of direct instruc- tion in science. There is a wealth of such evidence. A series of reviews by the U.S. National Academy of Sciences has re- cently described the results of experiments that provide evi- dence for the negative consequences of unguided science in- struction at all age levels and across a variety of science and math content. McCray, DeHaan, and Schuck (2003) re- viewed studies and practical experience in the education of college undergraduates in engineering, technology, science, and mathematics. Gollub, Berthanthal, Labov, and Curtis (2003) reviewed studies and experience teaching science and mathematics in high school. Kilpatrick, Swafford, and Findell (2001) reported studies and made suggestions for ele- mentary and middle school teaching of mathematics. Each of these and other publications by the U.S. National Academy of Sciences amply document the lack of evidence for un- guided approaches and the benefits of more strongly guided instruction. Most provide a set of instructional principles for educators that are based on solid research. These reports were prepared, in part, because of the poor state of science and mathematics education in the United States. Finally, in accord with the ATI findings and the expertise reversal effect, Roblyer, Edwards, and Havriluk (1997) reported that teach- ers have found that discovery learning is successful only when students have prerequisite knowledge and undergo some prior structured experiences.

Medical Problem-Based Learning Research
All in all, a lack of clarity about the difference between learn- ing a discipline and research in the discipline coupled with the priority afforded to unbiased observation in the best inductivist and empiricist tradition has led many educators to advocate a problem-based method as the way to teach a disci- pline (Allen, Barker, & Ramsden, 1986; Anthony, 1973; Bar- rows & Tamblyn, 1980; Obioma, 1986). Not only did PBL seem to mesh with ideas in, for example, the philosophy of science, but it also fit well with progressive learner-centered views emphasizing direct experience and individual inquiry. Cawthron and Rowell (1978) stated that it all seemed to fit. The logic of knowledge and the psychology of knowledge coalesced under the umbrella term discovery. Why, he asked, should educators look further than the traditional inductivist and empiricist explanation of the process?
In an attempt to rescue medical students from lectures and memory-based recall exams, approximately 60 medical schools in North America have adopted PBL in the past two decades. This variant of constructivist instruction with minimal guidance, introduced at the McMaster University School of Medicine in 1969, asks medical students to work in groups to diagnose and suggest treatment for common patient symptoms. PBL student groups are supervised by a clinical faculty member who is directed not to solve prob- lems for the students but instead to offer alternatives and suggest sources of information.
The best known survey of the comparisons of PBL with conventional medical school instruction was conducted by Albanese and Mitchell (1993). Their meta-analysis of the English language literature of the effectiveness of PBL pro- duced a number of negative findings concerning its impact, including lower basic science exam scores, no differences in residency selections, and more study hours each day. They reported that although PBL students receive better scores for their clinical performance, they also order significantly more unnecessary tests at a much higher cost per patient with less benefit. There was an indication in their review that increased clinical practice evaluation scores may have been due to the fact the PBL students are required to spend more time in clinical settings.
Berkson (1993) also reviewed much of the literature on PBL and arrived at many of the same conclusions as Albanese and Mitchell (1993). She reviewed studies where the prob- lem-solving ability of PBL students was compared with the same ability in conventionally trained students and found no support for any differences, and so failed to replicate the clini- cal advantage found by Albanese and Mitchell. Colliver (2000) reviewed existing studies comparing the effectiveness of PBL in medicine to conventional medical school curricula. He concluded that PBL studies show no statistical effect on the performance of medical students on standardized tests or on instructor-designed tests during the first 2 years of medical school. Also important for medical educators has been the constant finding in research summaries that PBL is not more effective but is more costly than traditional instruction. Of course, some supporters of PBL are aware of its limitations. Hmelo-Silver (2004) placed strong question marks concern- ing the general validity of PBL. According to her,
Certain aspects of the PBL model should be tailored to the developmental level of the learners ... there may be a place for direct instruction on a just-in-time basis. In other words, as students are grappling with a problem and confronted with the need for particular kinds of knowledge, a lecture at the right time may be beneficial. ... Some techniques such as procedural facilitation, scripted cooperation, and structured journals may prove useful tools in moving PBL to other set- tings. (pp. 260–261)
Two major components of PBL are the explicit teaching of problem-solving strategies in the form of the hypothetico-de- ductive method of reasoning (Barrows & Tamblyn, 1980), and teaching of basic content in the context of a specific case or instance. Proponents argue that problem-centered educa- tion is superior to conventional education. Students taught problem-solving skills, in particular through the use of the hypothetico-deductive method, and given problems to prac- tice those skills learn in a more meaningful way. It is assumed that because students are exposed to problems from the be- ginning, they have more opportunity to practice these skills, and that by explicitly applying the hypothetico-deductive method they learn to analyze problems and search for expla- nations, improving their comprehension of clinical problems (Norman & Schmidt, 1992). Patel and colleagues argued that the hypothetico-deductive method may not be the most effi- cient way of solving clinical problems (Patel & Groen, 1986; Patel, Arocha, & Kaufman, 1994).
In the medical domain, Patel, Groen, and Norman (1993) showed that teaching basic science within a clinical context may have the disadvantage that once basic science knowl- edge is contextualized, it is difficult to separate it from the particular clinical problems into which it has been integrated. They showed that students trained in a PBL curriculum failed to separate basic science knowledge from the specific clini- cal knowledge associated with particular patients. Although PBL students generated more elaborate explanations, they had less coherent explanations and more errors. If students have difficulty separating the biomedical knowledge they have learned from the particular clinical cases associated with that knowledge, it is not surprising that when given a different problem they bring to bear on the new problem some irrelevant biomedical knowledge.
This appears to persist after training. In a study of the ef- fect of undergraduate training in PBL—as opposed to a con- ventional curriculum—on the performance of residents on
the organization of clinical and biomedical knowledge and the use of reasoning strategies, Arocha and Patel (1995) found that participants trained in PBL retained the back- ward-directed reasoning pattern, but did not seem to acquire forward-directed reasoning, which is a hallmark of expertise. This finding means that something in PBL may hinder the de- velopment of the forward reasoning pattern.
Experts use schema-based pattern recognition to deter- mine the cause of a patient’s illness. According to Elstein (1994) knowledge organization and schema acquisition are more important for the development of expertise than the use of particular methods of problem solving. In this re- gard, cognitive research has shown that to achieve expertise in a domain, learners must acquire the necessary schemata that allow them to meaningfully and efficiently interpret in- formation and identify the problem structure. Schemata ac- complish this by guiding the selection of relevant informa- tion and the screening out of irrelevant information.
Arocha and Patel (1995) concluded that the negative results
can be accounted for by the effect of splitting of attention resources and the high working memory load on schema acquisition during problem solving. In solving clinical problems, subjects must attend to the current diagnostic hy- pothesis, the data in the problem presented to them, and any intermediate hypothesis between the diagnosis and the pa- tient data (e.g., a pathophysiological process underlying the signs and symptoms). If we consider that more than one hy- pothesis has been generated, the cognitive resources needed for maintaining this information in working memory must be such that few cognitive resources are left for acquiring the problem schema. Although problems can be solved suc- cessfully using the hypothetico-deductive method, the scar- city of attentional and memory resources may result in the students having difficulties learning problem schemata in an adequate manner. It is possible to hypothesize that one of the reasons for the failure of PBLC subjects to acquire a forward-directed reasoning style as found in this study may be the use of problem solving strategies, such as the hypothetico-deductive method, as a learning strategy.
This is completely in line with our claim that the epistemol- ogy of a discipline should not be confused with a pedagogy for teaching or learning it. The practice of a profession is not the same as learning to practice the profession.

CONCLUSIONS
After a half-century of advocacy associated with instruction using minimal guidance, it appears that there is no body of research supporting the technique. In so far as there is any evidence from controlled studies, it almost uniformly sup- ports direct, strong instructional guidance rather than constructivist-based minimal guidance during the instruc- tion of novice to intermediate learners. Even for students
with considerable prior knowledge, strong guidance while learning is most often found to be equally effective as un- guided approaches. Not only is unguided instruction nor- mally less effective; there is also evidence that it may have negative results when students acquire misconceptions or incomplete or disorganized knowledge.
Although the reasons for the ongoing popularity of a failed approach are unclear, the origins of the support for in- struction with minimal guidance in science education and medical education might be found in the post-Sputnik sci- ence curriculum reforms such as Biological Sciences Curric- ulum Study, Chemical Education Material Study, and Physi- cal Science Study Committee. At that time, educators shifted away from teaching a discipline as a body of knowledge to- ward the assumption that knowledge can best or only be learned through experience that is based only on the proce- dures of the discipline. This point of view appears to have led to unguided practical or project work and the rejection of in- struction based on the facts, laws, principles, and theories that make up a discipline’s content. The emphasis on the practical application of what is being learned seems very pos- itive. However, it may be an error to assume that the peda- gogic content of the learning experience is identical to the methods and processes (i.e., the epistemology) of the disci- pline being studied and a mistake to assume that instruction should exclusively focus on application. It is regrettable that current constructivist views have become ideological and of- ten epistemologically opposed to the presentation and expla- nation of knowledge. As a result, it is easy to share the puz- zlement of Handelsman et al. (2004), who, when discussing science education, asked: “Why do outstanding scientists who demand rigorous proof for scientific assertions in their research continue to use and, indeed defend on the bias of in- tuition alone, teaching methods that are not the most effec- tive?” (p. 521). It is also easy to agree with Mayer’s (2004) recommendation that we “move educational reform efforts from the fuzzy and unproductive world of ideology—which sometimes hides under the various banners of constructivism—to the sharp and productive world of the- ory-based research on how people learn” (p. 18).
REFERENCES
Albanese, M., & Mitchell, S. (1993). Problem-based learning: A review of the literature on its outcomes and implementation issues. Academic Medi- cine, 68, 52–81.
Allen, J. B., Barker, L. N., & Ramsden, J. H. (1986). Guided inquiry labora- tory. Journal of Chemical Education, 63, 533–534.
Anderson, J. R. (1996). ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. Ameri- can Psychologist, 51, 355–365.
Anthony, W. S. (1973). Learning to discover rules by discovery. Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 325–328.
Arocha, J. F., & Patel, V. L. (1995). Novice diagnostic reasoning in medicine: Accounting for clinical evidence. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4, 355–384.
Atkinson, R., & Shiffrin, R. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. Spence & J. Spence (Eds.), The psychol- ogy of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic.
Aulls, M. W. (2002). The contributions of co-occurring forms of classroom discourse and academic activities to curriculum events and instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 520–538.
Ausubel, D. P. (1964). Some psychological and educational limitations of learning by discovery. The Arithmetic Teacher, 11, 290–302.
Barrows, H. S., & Tamblyn, R. M. (1980). Problem-based learning: An ap- proach to medical education. New York: Springer.
Berkson, L. (1993). Problem-based learning: Have the expectations been met? Academic Medicine, 68(Suppl.), S79–S88.
Bernstein, D. A., Penner, L. A., Clarke-Stewart, A., Roy, E. J., & Wickens, C. D. (2003). Psychology (6th ed.). Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Boud, D., Keogh, R., & Walker, D. (Eds.). (1985). Reflection: Turning expe- rience into learning. London: Kogan Page.
Brown, A., & Campione, J. (1994). Guided discovery in a community of learners. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 229–270). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1961). The art of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31, 21–32.
Carlson, R. A., Lundy, D. H., & Schneider, W. (1992). Strategy guidance and memory aiding in learning a problem-solving skill. Human Factors, 34, 129–145.
Carroll, W. (1994). Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 360–367. Cawthron, E. R., & Rowell, J. A. (1978). Epistemology and science educa-
tion. Studies in Science Education, 5, 51–59. Chall, J. S. (2000). The academic achievement challenge. New York:
Guilford. Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psy-
chology, 4, 55–81. Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R.
Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (pp.
7–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Clark, R. E. (1982). Antagonism between achievement and enjoyment in
ATI studies. Educational Psychologist, 17, 92–101. Clark, R. E. (1989). When teaching kills learning: Research on
mathemathantics. In H. N. Mandl, N. Bennett, E. de Corte, & H. F. Freidrich (Eds.), Learning and instruction: European research in an in- ternational context (Vol. 2, pp. 1–22). London: Pergamon.
Clark, R. E., & Estes, F. (1998). Technology or craft: What are we doing? Educational Technology, 38(5), 5–11.
Clark, R. E., & Estes, F. (1999). The development of authentic educational technologies. Educational Technology, 37(2), 5–16.
Colliver, J. A. (2000). Effectiveness of problem-based learning curricula: Research and theory. Academic Medicine, 75, 259–266.
Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educa- tional Psychology, 79, 347–362.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A recon- sideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114.
Craig, R. (1956). Directed versus independent discovery of established rela- tions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 47, 223–235.
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. New York: Irvington.
De Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague, Nether- lands: Mouton. (Original work published 1946)
Dehoney, J. (1995). Cognitive task analysis: Implications for the theory and practice of instructional design. Proceedings of the Annual National Con- vention of the Association for Educational Communications and Technol- ogy (AECT), 113–123. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 383 294)
Downloaded At: 14:04 17 June 2011
MINIMAL GUIDANCE 85
Egan, D. E., & Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic draw- ings. Memory and Cognition, 7, 149–158.
Elstein, A. S. (1994). What goes around comes around: Return of the hypothetico-deductive strategy. Teaching & Learning in Medicine, 6, 121–123.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psy- chological Review, 102, 211–245.
Estes, F., & Clark, R. E. (1999). Authentic educational technologies: The lynchpin between theory and practice. Educational Technology, 37(6), 5–13.
Glaser, R. (1987). Further notes toward a psychology of instruction. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Gollub, J. P., Berthanthal, M., Labov, J., & Curtis, C. (Eds.). (2003). Learning and understanding: Improving advanced study of mathematics and science in U.S. high schools. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Handelsman, J., Egert-May, D., Beichner, R., Bruns, P., Change, A., DeHaan, R., et al. (2004). Scientific teaching. Science, 304, 521–522. Hardiman, P., Pollatsek, A., & Weil, A. (1986). Learning to understand the
balance beam. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 1–30. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how do stu-
dents learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16, 235–266. Hodson, D. (1988). Experiments in science and science teaching. Educa-
tional Philosophy and Theory, 20, 53–66. Hurd, P. D. (1969). New directions in teaching secondary school science.
Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. Iliff, C. H. (1994). Kolb’s learning style inventory: A meta-analysis. Unpub-
lished doctoral dissertation, Boston University, Boston. Jeffries, R., Turner, A., Polson, P., & Atwood, M. (1981). Processes involved in designing software. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 255–283). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Inc. Jonassen, D. (1991). Objectivism vs. constructivism. Educational Technol-
ogy Research and Development, 39(3), 5–14. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). Expertise reversal
effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem
solving is superior to studying worked examples. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93, 579–588. Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (Eds.). (2001). Adding it up:
Helping children learn mathematics. Washington, DC: National Acad-
emies Press. Kirschner, P. A. (1991). Practicals in higher science education. Utrecht,
Netherlands: Lemma. Kirschner, P. A. (1992). Epistemology, practical work and academic skills in
science education. Science and Education, 1, 273–299. Kirschner, P. A., Martens, R. L., & Strijbos, J.-W. (2004). CSCL in higher education? A framework for designing multiple collaborative environ- ments. In P. Dillenbourg (Series Ed.) & J.-W. Strijbos, P. A. Kirschner, & R. L. Martens (Vol. Eds.), Computer-supported collaborative learning: Vol. 3. What we know about CSCL ... and implementing it in higher edu-
cation (pp. 3–30). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic. Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early
science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning.
Psychological Science, 15, 661–667. Kolb, D. A. (1971). Individual learning styles and the learning process
(Working Paper No. 535–71). Cambridge, MA: Sloan School of Manage-
ment, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Kolb, D. A. (1976). The learning style inventory: Technical manual. Boston,
MA: McBer. Kolb, D. A. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the source of
learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kolb, D. A. (1999). Learning Style Inventory, version 3. Boston: TRG
Hay/McBer, Training Resources Group. Kolb, D. A., Boyatzis, R. E., & Mainemelis, C. (2001). Experiential learning
theory: Previous research and new directions. In R. J. Sternberg & L.
Zhang (Eds.), Perspectives on thinking, learning, and cognitive styles. The educational psychology series (pp. 227–247). Mahwah, NJ: Law- rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Kolb, D. A., & Fry, R. (1975). Toward an applied theory of experiential learning. In C. Cooper (Ed.), Studies of group process (pp. 33–57). New York: Wiley.
Kyle, W. C., Jr. (1980). The distinction between inquiry and scientific in- quiry and why high school students should be cognizant of the distinction. Journal of Research on Science Teaching, 17, 123–130.
Kyllonen, P. C., & Lajoie, S. P. (2003). Reassessing aptitude: Introduction to a special issue in honor of Richard E. Snow. Educational Psychologist, 38, 79–83.
Mayer, R. (2001). Multi-media learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni- versity Press.
Mayer, R. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychol- ogist, 59, 14–19.
McCray, R., DeHaan, R. L., & Schuck, J. A. (Eds.). (2003). Improving un- dergraduate instruction in science, technology, engineering, and mathe- matics: Report of a workshop. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
McKeough, A., Lupart, J., & Marini, A. (Eds.). (1995). Teaching for trans- fer: Fostering generalization in learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Miller, C., Lehman, J., & Koedinger, K. (1999). Goals and learning in microworlds. Cognitive Science, 23, 305–336.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Moreno, R. (2004). Decreasing cognitive load in novice students: Effects of explanatory versus corrective feedback in discovery-based multimedia. Instructional Science, 32, 99–113.
Nadolski, R. J., Kirschner, P. A., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2005). Opti- mising the number of steps in learning tasks for complex skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 223–237.
Norman, G. R., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). The psychological basis of prob- lem-based learning: A review of the evidence. Academic Medicine, 67, 557–565.
Novak, J. D. (1988). Learning science and the science of learning. Studies in Science Education, 15, 77–101.
Obioma, G. O. (1986). Expository and guided discovery methods of present- ing secondary school physics. European Journal of Science Education, 8, 51–56.
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psy- chology, 84, 429–434.
Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and in- structional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.
Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: Instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cogni- tive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 1–8.
Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive-load ap- proach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York: Basic Books.
Patel, V. L., Arocha, J. F., & Kaufman, D. R. (1994). Diagnostic reasoning and expertise. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, 31, 137–252.
Patel, V. L., & Groen, G. J. (1986). Knowledge-based solution strategies in medical reasoning. Cognitive Science, 10, 91–116.
Patel, V. L., Groen, G. J., & Norman, G. R. (1993). Reasoning and instruc- tion in medical curricula. Cognition & Instruction, 10, 335–378.
Perkins, D. N. (1991). Technology meets constructivism: Do they make a marriage? Educational Technology, 13, 18–23.
Downloaded At: 14:04 17 June 2011
86 KIRSCHNER, SWELLER, CLARK
Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. (1959). Short-term retention of individual ver- bal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.
Pillay, H. (1994). Cognitive load and mental rotation: Structuring ortho- graphic projection for learning and problem solving. Instructional Sci- ence, 22, 91–113.
Quilici, J. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1996). Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 88, 144–161.
Roblyer, M. D. (1996). The constructivist/objectivist debate: Implications for instructional technology research. Learning and Leading With Tech- nology, 24, 12–16.
Roblyer, M. D., Edwards, J., & Havriluk, M. A. (1997). Integrating educa- tional technology into teaching (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Ruble, T. L., & Stout, D. E. (1993, March). Learning styles and end-user training: An unwarranted leap of faith. MIS Quarterly, 17, 115–117.
Rutherford, F. J. (1964). The role of inquiry in science teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2, 80–84.
Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 31–57.
Schmidt, H. G. (l983). Problem-based learning: Rationale and description. Medical Education, 17, 11–16.
Schmidt, H. G. (1998). Problem-based learning: Does it prepare medical students to become better doctors? The Medical Journal of Australia, 168, 429–430.
Schmidt, H. G. (2000). Assumptions underlying self-directed learning may be false. Medical Education, 34, 243–245.
Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teach- ing. Educational Researcher, 15, 4–14.
Shulman, L. S., & Hutchings, P. (1999, September–October). The scholar- ship of teaching: New elaborations, new developments. Change, 11–15.
Shulman, L., & Keisler, E. (Eds.). (1966). Learning by discovery: A critical appraisal. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Snow, R. E., Corno, L., & Jackson, D. N., III. (1994). Individual differences in conation: Selected constructs and measures. In H. F. O’Neil & M. Drillings (Eds.), Motivation: Theory and research (pp. 71–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Snow, R. E., Corno, L., & Jackson, D. (1996). Individual differences in af- fective and conative functions. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Hand- book of educational psychology (pp. 243–310). New York: Simon & Schuster.
Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. F. (1984). Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 347–376. Steffe, L., & Gale, J. (Eds.). (1995). Constructivism in education. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learn-
ing. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285. Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. Camberwell,
Australia: ACER Press. Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. Ross
(Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 215–266).
San Diego, CA: Academic. Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between
evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instruc-
tional Science, 32, 9–31. Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute
for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89. Sweller, J., Mawer, R., & Howe, W. (1982). The consequences of his- tory-cued and means-ends strategies in problems solving. American Jour-
nal of Psychology, 95, 455–484. Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architec-
ture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10,
251–296. Tarmizi, R., & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem
solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 424–436. Trafton, J. G., & Reiser, R. J. (1993). The contribution of studying examples and solving problems to skill acquisition. In M. Polson (Ed.), Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.
1017–1022). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Tuovinen, J. E., & Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load associ- ated with discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 91, 334–341. Van Joolingen, W. R., de Jong, T., Lazonder, A. W., Savelsbergh, E. R., &
Manlove, S. (2005). Co-Lab: Research and development of an online learning environment for collaborative scientific discovery learning. Computers in Human Behavior, 21, 671–688.
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1997). Training complex cognitive skills. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications.
Ward, M., & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 1–39.
Winn, W. (2003). Research methods and types of evidence for research in educational psychology. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 367–373.
Woltz, D. J. (2003). Implicit cognitive processes as aptitudes for learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 95–104.









A rebuttal to the criticisms of Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark

While there are critics of the Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark article, Sweller and his associates have written in their articles about:

  1. instructional designs for producing procedural learning (learning as behavior change) (Sweller, 1988);
  2. their grouping of seemingly disparate learning theories (Kirschner et al., 2006) and;
  3. a continuum of guidance beginning with worked examples that may be followed by practice, or transitioned to practice (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, and Sweller, 2003; Renkl, Atkinson, Maier, and Staley, 2002)

Kirschner et al. (2006) describe worked examples as an instructional design solution for procedural learning. Clark, Nguyen, and Sweller (2006) describe this as a very effective, empirically validated method of teaching learners procedural skill acquisition. Evidence for learning by studying worked-examples, is known as the worked-example effectand has been found to be useful in many domains [e.g. music, chess, athletics (Atkinson, Derry, Renkl, & Wortham, 2000); concept mapping (Hilbert & Renkl, 2007); geometry (Tarmizi and Sweller, 1988); physics, mathematics, or programming (Gerjets, Scheiter, and Catrambone, 2004)].

Kirschner et al. (2006) describe why they group a series of seemingly disparate learning theories (Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based learning). The reasoning for this grouping is because each learning theory promotes the same constructivist teaching technique -- "learning by doing." While they argue "learning by doing" is useful for more knowledgeable learners, they argue this constructivist teaching technique is not useful for novices. Mayer states that it promotes behavioral activity too early in the learning process, when learners should be cognitively active (Mayer, 2004).[15]

In addition, Sweller and his associates describe a continuum of guidance, starting with worked examples to slowly fade guidance. This continuum of faded guidance has been tested empirically to produce a series of learning effects: theworked-example effect (Sweller and Cooper, 1985), the guidance fading effect (Renkl, Atkinson, Maier, and Staley, 2002), and the expertise-reversal effect (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, and Sweller, 2003).

[edit]Criticism of discovery-based teaching techniques

After a half century of advocacy associated with instruction using minimal guidance, there appears no body of research supporting the technique. In so far as there is any evidence from controlled studies, it almost uniformly supports direct, strong instructional guidance rather constructivist-based minimal guidance during the instruction of novice to intermediate learners. Even for students with considerable prior knowledge, strong guidance while learning is most often found to be equally effective as unguided approaches. Not only is unguided instruction normally less effective; there is also evidence that it may have negative results when student acquire misconceptions or incomplete or disorganized knowledge
 
— Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching by Kirschner, Sweller, Clark

[16]

Mayer (2004)[15] argues against discovery-based teaching techniques and provides an extensive review to support this argument. Mayer's arguments are against pure discovery, and are not specifically aimed at constructivism: "Nothing in this article should be construed as arguing against the view of learning as knowledge construction or against using hands-on inquiry or group discussion that promotes the process of knowledge construction in learners. The main conclusion I draw from the three research literatures I have reviewed is that it would be a mistake to interpret the current constructivist view of learning as a rationale for reviving pure discovery as a method of instruction."[15]

Mayer's concern is how one applies discovery-based teaching techniques. He provides empirical research as evidence that discovery-based teaching techniques are inadequate. Here he cites this literature and makes his point “For example, a recent replication is research showing that students learn to become better at solving mathematics problems when they study worked-out examples rather than when they solely engage in hands-on problem solving (Sweller, 1999). Today’s proponents of discovery methods, who claim to draw their support from constructivist philosophy, are making inroads into educational practice. Yet a dispassionate review of the relevant research literature shows that discovery-based practice is not as effective as guided discovery.” (Mayer, 2004, p. 18)

Mayer’s point is that people often misuse constructivism to promote pure discovery-based teaching techniques. He proposes that the instructional design recommendations of constructivism are too often aimed at discovery-based practice (Mayer, 2004). Sweller (1988) found evidence that practice by novices during early schema acquisition, distracts these learners with unnecessary search-based activity, when the learner's attention should be focused on understanding (acquiring schemas).

The study by Kirschner et al. from which the quote at the beginning of this section was taken has been widely cited and is important for showing the limits of minimally-guided instruction.[17] Hmelo-Silver et al. responded,[18] pointing out that Kirschner et al. conflated constructivist teaching techniques such as inquiry learning with "discovery learning". (See the preceding two sections of this article.) This would agree with Mayer's viewpoint that even though constructivism as a theory and teaching techniques incorporating guidance are likely valid applications of this theory, nevertheless a tradition of misunderstanding has led to some question "pure discovery" techniques.

SOURCE:  http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Constructivism_(learning_theory)#A_rebuttal_to_the_criticisms_of_Kirschner.2C_Sweller.2C_and_Clark






Comments